Discover the world's research
- 20+ million members
- 135+ million publications
- 700k+ research projects
Join for free
Joumnal
of
Neurology,
Neurosurgery,
and
Psychiatry
1995;58:655-664
NEUROLOGICAL
INVESTIGATIONS
Neuropsychological
assessment
Lisa
Cipolotti,
Elizabeth
K
Warrington
Patients
with
brain
damage
may
present
with
impairments
of
memory,
language,
percep-
tion,
thought,
action,
and
other
functions.
These
cognitive
deficits
can
occur
both
in
multiple
domains
or
as
highly
selective
impairments.
In
the
19th
century
and
early
20th
century,
neurologists
investigated
cogni-
tive
impairments
in
patients
with
neurological
disease
by
clinical
and
descriptive
methods.
These
methods
provided
new
insights
and
allowed
the
isolation
of
distinct
syndromes-
for
example,
aphasia,'2
alexia
and
agraphia,3
acalculia,4
visual
agnosia,5
and
amnesia.6
Indeed,
these
discoveries
formed
the
basis
for
the
development
of
a
new
discipline,
"neu-
ropsychology",
devoted
to
the
study
of
the
relation
between
the
brain
and
cognitive
functions.
The
clinical
and
descriptive
meth-
ods,
however,
provided
a
poor
standard
of
description
of
the
cognitive
impairments
in
these
syndromes.
They
were
".
.
.
little
more
than
the
bald
statement
of
the
clinical
opin-
ion
of
the
investigator.
.
.".
To
deal
with
this
lack,
neuropsychologists
developed
principled
techniques
for
the
mea-
surement
of
cognitive
functioning.
In
the
early
days,
psychometric
tests,
originally
developed
for
the
measurement
of
either
scholastic
attainment
or
occupational
guid-
ance,
were
used.
In
particular,
tests
for
the
measurement
of
intellectual
and
memory
functions
became
available
to
the
clinician.8-1
Gradually,
over
the
past
four
decades
an
increasing
number
of
measurement
tools
have
been
specifically
designed
for
investigating
the
cognitive
functions
of
patients
with
sus-
pected
or
confirmed
cerebral
disease.
Neuropsychological
assessment
involves
the
use
of
a
series
of
tests
that
are
"reliable"-in
the
same
circumstances
they
produce
the
same
result-and
"valid"-they
measure
what
they
are
designed
to
measure.
The
aim
of
this
paper
is
to
provide
an
overview
of
the
main
methods
for
the
assess-
ment
of
cognitive
function
and
an
outline
of
what
may
prompt
a
neuropsychological
assessment
(see
also
Lezak,"2
Crawford
et
al,3
and
Hodges'4).
Before
approaching
a
neu-
ropsychological
assessment
it
is
necessary
to
have
a
general
theoretical
structure
on
which
to
base
and
interpret
the
different
levels
of
disturbance
that
can
arise
as
a
result
of
cere-
bral
damage.
(for
a
similar
view
see
Hodges'4)
In
the
next
three
sections
we
discuss
our
gen-
eral
theoretical
schema;
the
methods
of
assessment
of
cognitive
function;
and
the
pur-
poses
of
a
neuropsychological
examination.
General
theoretical
schema
Our
approach
makes
the
assumption
that
impairments
in
cognitive
function
can
best
be
studied
and
understood
by
(a)
assuming
that
there
is
a
high
degree
of
functional
specialisa-
tion
in
the
cerebral
cortex;
(b)
by
undertaking
a
modularity
approach
to
the
analysis
of
com-
plex
cognitive
skills;
and
(c)
by
assuming
that
brain
damage
can
selectively
disrupt
some
components
of
a
cognitive
system.'5
The
extent
to
which
these
assumptions
have
a
direct
anatomical
substrate
is
less
established.
The
idea
that
the
human
brain
is
highly
differentiated
in
terms
of
its
functional
organ-
isation
is
not
new.
The
phrenologists
in
the
early
19th
century
were
already
speculating
that
the
convoluted
surface
of
the
brain
reflected
the
juxtaposition
of
a
large
number
of
discrete
cerebral
organs
each
subserving
a
particular
psychological
faculty.'6
Several
years
after
these
accounts,
neurologists
began
to
study
and
record
impairments
of
the
higher
cortical
functions
and
their
accompa-
nying
cerebral
lesions.
Aphasic
disorders
were
extensively
studied
and
the
specialised
lan-
guage
functions
of
the
left
hemisphere
were
recognised.'2'7
Subsequently,
after
the
pio-
neering
work
of
Jackson,'8
the
specialised
visuoperceptual
functions
of
the
right
hemi-
sphere
were
also
recognised.
These
early
workers
not
only
localised
a
number
of
spe-
cialised
functions
in
the
brain
but
they
also
discussed
their
findings
within
a
theoretical
framework.
For
example,
Lichtheim'9
pro-
duced
a
complex
diagram
of
the
various
sub-
components
of
the
language
system
by
incorporating
and
expanding
on
Wemicke's2
original
scheme.
In
his
diagram,
the
various
subcomponents
of
the
language
functions
are
represented
as
a
series
of
"centres"
(for
exam-
ple,
the
concept
centre,
the centre
of
the
motor
images
of
the
words,
the
centre
of
the
auditory
images
of
the
words),
each
of
which
was
thought
to
be
located
in
a
specific
area
of
the
brain.
These
different
functional
centres
were
thought
to
be
connected
with
each
other
through
sets
of
fibre
tracts.
This
approach-
those
adopting
it
were
termed
the
"diagram
makers"-has
some
resemblance
to
that
of
Psychology
Department,
National
Hospital
for
Neurology
and
Neurosurgery,
Queen
Square,
London
WClN
3BG,
UK
L
Cipolotti
E
K
Warrington
Correspondence
to:
Professor
E
K
Warrington.
655
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
Cipolotti,
Warrington
modem
cognitive
neuropsychology
theorists.
Despite
this,
the
idea
that
cognitive
skills
such
as
language
could
consist
of
multicom-
ponents
and
be
localised
in
different,
highly
specialised
areas
of
the
brain
came
under
attack
from
the
"global
theorists".2'
Of
par-
ticular
relevance
here
is
the
development
of
"mass
action"
theories.
These
theories
pro-
posed
that
there
was
no
differentiation
in
the
cortex
for
specific
cognitive
functions;
rather,
that
it
was
equipotential
with
respect
to
cog-
nitive
abilities.24
According
to
such
a
view,
any
form
of
neurological
damage
would
deplete
by
a
greater
or
lesser
extent
the
avail-
able
amount
of
some
general
cognitive
resource
and
not
specific
cognitive
functions.
The
amount
of
damage
to
the
general
cogni-
tive
resource,
also
termed
intellect
or
abstract
attitude,
would
depend
on
the
extent
of
the
brain
damage
and
not
on
the
site
of
the
damage.
The
notion
that
different
brain
regions
are
specialised
for
different
cognitive
functions
regained
popularity
in
the
1950s
and
the
modem
revolution
in
imaging
techniques
has
made
it
possible
to
visualise
these
structures
in
the
living
human
brain.
The
idea
that
there
are
cognitive
processing
systems
that
involve
only
specialised
brain
regions
is
now
accepted
".
. .
as
one
of
the
comerstones
of
modem
brain
science
.".
.".25
For
about
95%
of
right
handers
and
70%
of
left
handers
major
lan-
guage,
literacy
(reading,
writing,
and
calcula-
tion),
verbal
short
term
memory,
verbal
long
term
memory,
semantic
memory,
and
praxis
are
represented
in
the
left
hemisphere.
The
right
hemisphere
is
involved
in
non-verbal
processing
such
as
the
analysis
of
perceptual
and
spatial
stimuli,
spatial
short
term
mem-
ory,
visual
long
term
memory,
spatially
directed
attention,
face
recognition,
topo-
graphical
knowledge,
and
in
some
prosodic
components
of
language.
For
those
few
peo-
ple
who
do
not
have
normal
lateralisation
this
pattern
seems
to
be
reversed,
although
a
very
small
proportion
of
subjects
may
have
bilat-
eral
organisation
of
some
cognitive
skills.
The
anterior
parts
of
both
hemispheres
have
been
accepted
as
being
implicated
in
problem
solv-
ing
processes
that
are
required
in
a
wide
range
of
situations
including
practical
rou-
tines
and
social
interactions
as
well
as
abstract
reasoning
tasks.7
The
most
posterior
parts
of
both
hemispheres
are
involved
in
early
visual
processing.
Subcortical,
as
well
as
cortical,
brain
regions,
are
involved
in
atten-
tion
and
alertness.
Subcortical
brain
regions
are
also
involved
in
episodic
memory,
in
some
aspects
of
long
term
memory,
and
in
the
motor
control
of
language.12
A
modularity
approach
to
the
analysis
of
cognitive
skills
implies
that
each
complex
cognitive
process
can
be
thought
of
as
con-
sisting
of
a
series
of
functionally
independent
specialised
subprocesses.72$29
The
interaction
of
these
subprocesses
results
in
the
complex
cognitive
skills.
The
way
in
which
the
cogni-
tive
processes
are
organised
is
often
charac-
terised,
similarly
to
the
"diagram
maker"
approach,
in
terms
of
flow
diagrams
that
attempt
to
detail
the
way
that
the
different
subprocesses
are
brought
together
to
perform
a
specific
task.
Empirical
support
for
the
modularity
approach
can
be
obtained
at
vari-
ous
levels
including
the
neurophysiological,
neuroanatomical,
and
neuropsychological.'031
For
example,
numeracy
has
been
fractionated
into
several
independent
components:
cogni-
tive
mechanisms
for
number
comprehension,
number
production,
arithmetical
fact
retrieval,
and
arithmetical
procedures.'2
The
idea
that
complex
cognitive
skills
are
carried
out
by
distinct
subprocesses
com-
bined
with
the
idea
that
there
are
highly
spe-
cialised
areas
in
the
brain,
has
led
many
cognitive
neuropsychologists
to
assume
that
a
cerebral
lesion
can
damage
only
some
sub-
processes
within
complex
cognitive
skills.
Indeed,
cognitive
neuropsychologists
have
succeeded
in
showing
many
dissociations
between
the
subcomponents
of
cognitive
skills
that
allow
valid
conclusions
about
the
nature
and
functions
of
the
impaired
process-
ing
components
to
be
drawn.
Methods
of
assessment
of
cognitive
functions
One
of
the
fundamental
principles
underlying
neuropsychological
assessment
is
to
establish
whether
the
subject
is
still
functioning
at
their
premorbid
optimal
level
or
whether
there
has
been
a
deterioration.
Therefore,
the
methods
used
for
assessing
cognitive
functioning
in
neuropsychology
need
to
be
able
to
provide:
(a)
an
indirect
measure
of
the
premorbid
skills
of
a
person
and
(b)
a
measure
of
the
present
cognitive
state
of
that
person.
Once
the
two
types
of
measures
are
obtained
they
can
be
compared.
This
should
indicate:
firstly,
whether
the
functioning
has
changed
from
the
premorbid
state;
and
secondly
whether
this
reflects
organic
or
functional
impairment.
If
the
results
indicate
organic
impairment
then
an
attempt
will
be
made
to
establish
the
extent
of
the
change.
It
is
not
only
useful
to
know
that
a
change
in
cognitive
functioning
has
occurred;
it
is
also
useful
to
know
whether
the
change
can
be
charac-
terised
as
global
or
focal.
If
the
cognitive
impairment
is
focal,
neuropsychological
mea-
sures
can
be
used
to
specify
more
precisely
the
cognitive
impairments:
whether
the
impairment
is
indicative
of
lateralised
dys-
function
or
confined
to
the
anterior
or
poste-
rior
regions
of
the
brain.
In
exceptional
cases,
it
is
possible
to
document
highly
selective
cognitive
impairments
with
a
known
and
rela-
tively
precise
anatomical
localisation.
A
comprehensive
neuropsychological
examination
would
include
the
assessment
of:
(a)
premorbid
ability;
(b)
general
intellec-
tual
level;
(c)
memory;
(d)
language;
(e)
calculation;
(f)
problem
solving;
(g)
alertness
and
attention;
(h)
visual
and
space
percep-
tion.
Ideally
a
cognitive
profile
would
be
constructed
from
performance
on
tests
of
proved
validity
and
comparable
difficulty.
A
long
term
aim
for
the
neuropsychologist
is
to
achieve
a
level
of
measurement
for
all
656
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
Neuropsychological
assessment
Verbl
IQ
Speed
..,..
Pe
rforma
nce
IQ
.I
\Verba
memory,/
Naming
Pe
Reading
_
Spelling
Spatial
skills
Arithmetic
Percentiles
<1
a1<5
a5<25
E
>25<50-
¢50O
cognitive
skills
that
permit
comparison
across
tasks
and
is
sensitive
to
change.
The
method
described
by
Newman
et
al
in
their
study
that
monitored
subjects
at
risk
for
Alzheimer's
dis-
ease
exploited
this
methodology
(figure)."
In
the
next
section
we
do
not
attempt
to
describe
all
the
tests
and
techniques
available
to
the
neuropsychologists
for
investigation
of
all
these
different
areas
of
cognition.
Rather
we
focus
on
three
main
areas
of
cognitive
function:
intelligence,
memory,
and
language
functions.
These
serve
to
illustrate
the
range
of
techniques
and
procedures
available
for
the
investigation
of
cognitive
impairments."
ASSESSMENT
OF
PREMORBID
ABILITY
Various
procedures
are
adopted
for
obtaining
an
indirect
measure
of
a
subject's
premorbid
skills,
which
can
then
be
compared
with
his
current
level
of
performance.
These
proce-
dures
can
be
divided
into
two
main
types:
methods
that
use
demographic
data
such
as
age,
sex,
race,
education,
and
occupation;
and
methods
that
use
tests
considered
to
be
relatively
resistant
to
neurological
and
psychi-
atric
disorders.
The
first
type
of
method
is
based
on
the
known
relation
between
a
num-
ber
of
demographic
variables
and
measured
IQ*.34
Not
only
may
educational
and
occupa-
tional
records
be used
as
a
rough
estimate
of
a
subject's
optimal
or
premorbid
level
of
functioning;
they
may
also,
through
the
use
of
various
types
of
regression
equations,
provide
a
more
precise
and
objective
estimate.'5
One
of
the
principle
limitations
of
this
type
of
technique,
however,
is
that
educational
and
occupational
histories
may
not
always
be
readily
available
and
they
may
be
incomplete,
uninformative,
or
anomalous.
The
second
method
involves
the
measure-
ment
of
a
cognitive
skill
that
is
known
to
be
highly
correlated
with
intellectual
factors
and
resistant
to
brain
damage.
This
method
is
obviously
not
reliant
on
preexisting
data.
Some
of
the
first
methods
of
this
type
involved
the
use
of
vocabulary.'6
These
meth-
ods
were
based
on
the
finding
that
patients
with
brain
disease
retained
old,
well
estab-
lished
verbal
skills,
such
as
those
implicated
in
the
verbal
definition
of
words,
long
after
other
cognitive
skills
were
impaired.
The
application
of
the
same
principle
led
to
the
development
of
various
Wechsler
deteriora-
tion
indices.'2
More
recently,
a
measure
of
premorbid
optimal
level
of
functioning
has
been
based
on
the
overlearned
skill
of
reading.
Nelson
and
McKenna"7
first
established
that
word
reading
skill,
as
measured
by
the
Schonell
graded
word
reading
test38
was
highly
corre-
lated
with
general
intelligence
in
a
normal
population.
Nelson
and
O'Connel139
then
established
that
the
reading
of
irregular
words,
such
as
"heir"
or
"chord",
which
can-
not
be
pronounced
correctly
by
applying
the
usual
rules
that
map
spelling
on
to
phonol-
ogy,
were
better
indicators
of
premorbid
intelligence
(IQ)
in
demented
subjects
than
estimates
based
on
reading
regular
words.
Nelson
subsequently
developed
the
National
adult
reading
test
(NART),
which
consists
of
50
irregular
words.
Indeed,
the
NART
has
become
one
of
the
most
commonly
used
measures
of
premorbid
intelligence.4'
An
American
version
of
this
test
is
also
avail-
able.42
One
of
the
major
limitations
of
the
NART
test
is
that
it
cannot
be
used
with
those
who
have
poor
literacy
skills
or
in
patients
with
obvious
impairments
of
speech
production
or
problems
associated
with
dyslexia.
In
addition,
there
have
been
claims
that
patients
with
dementing
disorders
may
not
present
with
preserved
irregular
word
reading.43
Consequently
these
patients
pre-
sent
with
difficulties
in
reading
the
NART
words,
and
this
would
result
in
erroneous
low
estimates
of
their
premorbid
IQ.
In
the
cases
of
early
dementia
when
language
skills
are
rel-
atively
unimpaired,
however,
it
has
been
shown
that
the
NART
remains
stable
over
time
and
can
be
used
as
a
predictor
of
the
premorbid
optimal
level
(Paque
and
Warrington,
unpublished
data).
ASSESSMENT
OF
GENERAL
INTELLECTUAL
LEVEL
Historically,
intelligence
has
been
defined
in
many
different
ways.
For
example,
Spearman,44
although
he
himself
avoided
the
term
intelligence,
proposed
the
existence
of
a
central
intellectual
ability,
which
he
referred
to
as
"g".
Although
he
never
actually
defined
what
g
was
he
thought
that
it
involved
"the
eduction
of
relations
and
correlates".4"
In
his
formulation
g
referred
to
the
determinant
of
shared
variance
among
various
tests
of
intel-
lectual
ability.
An
alternative
view,
associated
with
Thurstone45
and
Guilford,46
involved
the
application
of
the
term
intelligence
to
a
large
set
of
diverse
mental
abilities
(or
fac-
tors).
These
included
not
only
reasoning
and
problem
solving
on
new
data
but
also
*The
term
IQ
was
first
introduced
by
Stern40
to
describe
a
method
of
comparing
one
child's
score
on
the
Binet
intelligence
scale
with
the
performance
of
average
children
of
the
same
age.
It
is
nowadays
used
to
indicate
intellectual
level
by
comparing
a
subject's
performance
with
the
average
scores
attained
by
mem-
bers
of
the
same
age
group.
Example
of
a
circle
diagram
of
cognitive
skills.
Sectors
of
the
circle
have
been
apportioned
to
each
of
the
cognitive
skills
examined.
The
concentric
circles
represent
the
level
of
functioning
in
terms
of
percentile
score.
The
levels
of
the
subject's
test
performance
are
indicated
by
the
degree
of
eccentricity
within
a
sector.
In
this
patient
the
most
prominent
feature
was
a
global
memory
impairment
more
pronounced
for
verbal
than
visual
material.
(Diagram
courtesy
of
Newman
et
al.
33)
657
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
Cipolotti,
Wanington
specialised
knowledge
derived
from
prior
schooling
or
experience.
In
line
with
this,
Cattell47
distinguished
between
fluid
intelli-
gence,
the
ability
to
deal
with
novelty
and
to
adapt
one's
thinking
to
a
new
cognitive
prob-
lem,
and
crystallised
intelligence,
which
reflects
a
knowledge
base
and
skills
that
have
been
previously
acquired
through
learning
and
experience.
The
available
measures
of
intelligence
reflect
these
different
formulations
of
the
abil-
ities
underlying
intelligence.
For
example,
Raven's
test,
including
the
coloured
progres-
sive
matrices,"1
the
standard
progressive
matrices,'0
and
the
advanced
progressive
matrices
(sets
I
and
II)48,
are
widely
used
for
the
clinical
assessment
of
general
intelligence.
The
various
versions
of
this
test
are
believed
to
weigh
heavily
on
g
and
measure
processes
that
are
central
to
the
definition
of
fluid
intel-
ligence
(more
recently
also
termed
analytic
intelligence49).
Indeed,
they
require
abstract
reasoning,
induction
of
relations,
and
educ-
tion49
The
test
was
developed
as
a
"culture
fair"
measure
of
general
intellectual
ability
and
because
of
its
non-verbal
format,
its
ease
of
use,
and
its
speed
(especially
the
coloured
progressive
matrices),
it
has
gained
wide
use
in
both
clinical
and
research
settings.
This
may
be
overoptimistic
as
educational
level
has
subsequently
been
shown
to
have
a
major
effect
on
the
normal
subject's
performance.50
The
Wechsler
adult
intelligence
scale
(WAIS)5'
52
iS
considered
to
be
one
of
the
core
measures
for
evaluating
general
intellectual
ability.
It
involves
six
verbal
and
five
non-
verbal
subtests
that
sample
various
skills.
These
subtests
are
thought
to
measure
vari-
ous
mental
abilities
as
would
follow
from
Thurstone's45
and
Guilford's46
views,
includ-
ing
both
the
explicit
knowledge
base
derived
from
educational
and
previous
experience
and
the
ability
to
deal
with
and
solve
new
cognitive
problems.
Verbal
and
performance
IQs
are
determined
from
the
use
of
the
Wechsler
scales.
Much
research
has
focused
on
discrepancies
between
verbal
and
perfor-
mance
IQ
as
a
means
of
differentiating
between
left
and
right
hemisphere
impair-
ment53
although
this
has
not
resulted
in
a
general
consensus.'2
Indeed,
Warrington
et
al54
suggested
that
such
scales
have
little
value
as
regards
the
localisation
of
a
lesion
or,
for
that
matter,
the
identification
of
specific
cognitive
deficits.
Nevertheless,
the
Wechsler
adult
intelligence
scale-revised
(WAIS-R),
the
successor
of
the
WAIS
is
the
most
often
used
psychological
test
of
intellectual
functioning
and
is
a
cornerstone
for
most
neuropsycho-
logical
test
batteries.
It
is
also
widely
used
with
geriatric
patients,
and
recently,
norma-
tive
data
for
people
who
are
75
or
older
have
become
available.55
Numerous
studies
are
based
on
the
WAIS-R.
ASSESSMENT
OF
MEMORY
Memory
is
not
a
unitary
function
but
rather
a
collection
of
distinct
and
independent
com-
ponents,
each
of
which
is
associated
with
dif-
ferent
brain
structures.
A
broad
distinction
is
generally
made
between
short
and
long
term
memory.
Short
term
memory
is
considered
to
be
responsible
for
the
immediate
retention
of
a
limited
amount
of
information;
this
infor-
mation
will
decay
in
a
matter
of
seconds
if
it
is
not
refreshed.
Long
term
memory
retains
larger
amounts
of
information
for
longer
peri-
ods-depending
on
the
salience-which
may
be
for
minutes,
days,
and
years.
Short
and
long
term
memory
functions
can
be
further
divided
into
verbal
and
visual
memory
according
to
whether
they
retain
verbal
or
non-verbal
information.56
Long
term
memory
is
also
subdivided
into
implicit
(or
proce-
dural)
and
explicit
(or
declarative)
memory.57
Implicit
memory
retains
information
that
affects
behaviour
but
it
is
not
available
for
conscious
recollection
(for
example,
motor
skills,
conditioned
reflexes,
priming).
A
fur-
ther
example
is
the
three
letter
word
stem
completion
task,
which
can
be
performed
by
guessing
rather
than
by
conscious
recall.58
In
this
task
patients
are
presented
with
a
list
of
words
and
their
retention
is
tested
either
by
standard
recall
and
recognition
techniques
or
by
presenting
the
first
three
letters
of
the
target
item
in
a
word
completion
task
(for
example,
"cha"-chair).
Explicit
memory
retains
information
that
can
be
consciously
accessed.
It
is
subdivided
into
episodic
and
semantic
memory.59
Episodic
memory
con-
tains
information
about
temporally
dated
episodes
or
events
and
temporospatial
rela-
tions
among
them
(for
instance,
this
can
be
for
both
autobiographical
memories
and
memories
of
an
artificial
event
such
as
a
word
list
or
short
stories).
Semantic
memory
con-
tains
our
organised
knowledge
of
concepts
and
facts
as
well
as
words
and
their
meanings
(for
example,
encyclopaedic
memories).
Most
clinical
assessments
focus
on
three
main
types
of
memory
functions:
short
term
memory,
episodic
memory,
and
semantic
memory.
Assessment
of
short
term
memory
The
assessment
of
verbal
short
term
memory
usually
requires
the
repetition
of
a
progres-
sively
lengthening
string
of
digits
(digit
span),
letters,
and
words.
The
normal
range
of
digits
is
five
to
nine.
Spatial
short
term
memory
can
be
assessed
with
the
Corsi
block
tapping
test.60
This
requires
the
subject
to
tap
a
pro-
gressively
lengthening
sequence
of
blocks.
Assessment
of
episodic
memory
Many
tests
and
batteries
are
available
for
the
assessment
of
episodic
memory.61
These
use
either
a
recall
or
recognition
paradigm
and
typically
assess
the
anterograde
(the
ability
to
acquire
new
information)
rather
than
the
ret-
rograde
component
(the
ability
to
recall
pre-
viously
learnt
material).
One
of
the
oldest
batteries
used
is
the
Wechsler
memory
scale
(and
the
Wechsler
memory
scale-revised),
which
requires
the
recall
of
both
complex
ver-
bal
material
(for
example,
short
stories)
and
visual
material
(for
example,
reproduction
of
geometrical
designs).
Some
of
its
subtests
are
not
dependent
on
memory
itself
but
rather
on
attentional
processes
(for
example,
mental
658
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
659
Neuropsychological
assessment
control
and
orientation).
Unfortunately
all
the
subtests
contribute
to
the
final
memory
quotient.
A
more
recently
developed
test
for
the
assessment
of
long
term
verbal
memory
is
the
adult
memory
and
information
processing
battery,62
which
has
many
similarities
to
the
Wechsler
memory
scale.
The
Rivermead
behavioural
memory
tests
consist
of
a
series
of
tests
held
to
have
ecological
validity.6'
A
task
that
is
also
very
often
used
for
assessing
verbal
anterograde
recall
is
word
list
learning
(for
example,
the
auditory-verbal
learning
test64).
For
the
assessment
of
non-verbal
anterograde
recall,
the
two
most
commonly
used
tests
are
the
Rey-Osterreith
complex
figure
test65
and
the
Benton
revised
visual
retention
test.66
Both
require
the
recall
of
geometric
figures.
Warrington67
developed
a
test
that
used
a
recognition
rather
than
a
recall
paradigm
(the
recognition
memory
test).
The
recognition
paradigm
was
chosen
because
it
is
possible
to
have
comparable
tests
of
verbal
and
visual
memory.
This
test
incorporates
the
verbal
and
non-verbal
dichotomy
by
having
separate
subtests
with
word
and
face
stimuli.
Age
cor-
rected
percentile
scores
of
a
large
standardisa-
tion
sample
are
available.
Validation
of
this
test
has
shown
that
patients
with
right
hemi-
spheric
lesions
are
impaired
on
the
visual
ver-
sion
and
patients
with
left
hemispheric
lesions
are
impaired
on
the
verbal
version.
It
has
also
been
shown
that
this
test
can
detect
minor
degrees
of
memory
deficit."
Clegg
and
Warrington68
have
also
recently
standardised
and
validated
four
"easy"
memory
tests
(three
recognition
memory
tests
and
a
word
paired
associate
learning
test)
for
older
adults
(64
and
older)
that
are
recommended
for
patients
in
whom
memory
impairment
is
suspected
but
whose
mental
state
(for
example,
poor
attention,
anxiety,
or
agitation)
precludes
longer
or
more
demanding
tests.
Most
tests
of
retrograde
verbal
and
visual
recall
have
been
devised
for
research
rather
than
clinical
purposes.
They
normally
test
recall
and
recognition
of
famous
names
and
famous
faces.
Perhaps
because
they
so
quickly
become
dated
their
standardisation
and
validation
are
problematic.
A
relatively
new
test
assessing
autobiographical
memory
is
an
exception
to
this
rule
(autobiographical
memory
interview69).
This
test
requires
the
recall
of
personal
remote
facts
and
incidents
from
three
epochs:
childhood,
early
adult
life,
and
recent
experience.
Assessment
of
semantic
memory
Patients
with
a
semantic
memory
disorder
present
a
general
loss
of
knowledge,
including
object
and
word
meaning.
This
deficit
can
manifest
itself
as
an
inability
to
comprehend
words
and
identify
pictures
and
objects.
The
classic
syndromes
of
transcortical
sensory
aphasia
and
visual
associative
agnosia
have
been
identified
with
the
impairment
of
semantic
memory.70
There
are
no
standard-
ised
batteries
for
the
assessment
of
semantic
memory
because,
unlike
episodic
memory,
it
has
only
been
studied
in
the
past
20
years,
after
the
seminal
paper
of
Tulving.59
Semantic
memory
can,
however,
be
assessed
through
tests
devised
for
other
domains
(mainly
tests
also
used
for
the
assessment
of
language
dis-
orders).
To
evaluate
the
difficulties
in
word
definition
some
verbal
subtests
of
the
Wechsler
scales,
such
as
vocabulary
and
information,
can
be
used.
Naming
tests
can
be
used
as
indirect
evidence
of
semantic
memory
impairment
(see
language
section
later).
The
pyramids
and
palm
tree
test7'
was
developed
specifically
to
evaluate
impair-
ments
in
the
understanding
of
concepts.
There
is
a
verbal
and
a
pictorial
version
of
this
task
devised
for
assessing
conceptual
relations.
Limited
normative
data
are
avail-
able.
A
further
test,
the
British
picture
vocab-
ulary
test,72
which
uses
a
word
picture
matching
technique,
was
first
developed
for
the
assessment
of
language
developments
between
the
ages
2
and
18.
More
recently
it
has
been
standardised
in
a
normal
healthy
elderly
population
(Clegg
and
Warrington,
unpublished
data).
ASSESSMENT
OF
LANGUAGE
Language
is
not
a
unitary
fimction.
The
most
useful
dichotomy
is
to
consider
spoken
and
written
language
separately.
Spoken
language
can
be
characterised
as
a
collection
of
independent
components,
each
of
which
is
associated
with
different
brain
structures.
The
three
main
central
linguistic
components
are
phonology,
syntax,
and
semantics.7'
Phonological
processing
analyses
the constituent
sounds
of
words.
Syntactic
processing
analyses
the
grammatical
aspects
of
language-for
example,
the
ordering
of
the
individual
words
in
the
sentence.
Semantic
processing
analyses
the
referential
meaning
of
words.
In
addition
to
these
three
components
there
are
more
specialised
peripheral
systems
subserving
articulation
and
prosody.
Furthermore,
at
least
for
phonology
and
syn-
tax,
receptive
and
expressive
deficits
can
occur
as
selective
impairments.7'76
Disruptions
in
phonological
or
semantic
pro-
cessing
are
found
at
the
level
of
single
words
whereas
disruptions
in
syntactic
processing
are
found
at
the
level
of
sentences.
Assessment
of
spoken
language
There
are
several
traditional
clinical
tax-
onomies
of
the
acquired
aphasias
principally
inherited
from
the
earliest
scientific
papers
on
language
disorder.'-3
19
These
taxonomies,
based
on
mixed
functional,
anatomical,
and
pathological
terms,
have
inspired
the
develop-
ment
of
classic
aphasia
batteries.
The
most
widely
used
are
the
Boston
diagnostic
aphasia
examination,76
the
western
aphasia
battery,77
the
Porch
index
of
communicative
ability,78
and
the
Aachen
aphasia
test.79
The
traditional
taxonomies
that
form
their
basis
have
been
questioned
in
so
far
as
they
failed
to
capture
the
multidimensional
pattern
of
language
breakdown,
they
are
not
useful
for
guiding
therapy
or
for
the
detailed
analysis
and
understanding
of
language
disorders.80
In
this
section
we
provide
a
brief
account
of
the
core
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
Cipoloti,
Wamngton
tests
that
could
provide
a
framework
for
the
more
detailed
assessment
of
a
patient's
lan-
guage
impairment.
We
discuss
only
two
areas
of
language
dysfunction:
word
and
sentence
comprehension
and
word
and
sentence
retrieval.
Word
and
sentence
comprehension-Word
comprehension
deficits
can
occur
as
a
result
of
an
impairment
in
auditory
perception
or
as
a
result
of
an
impairment
in
word
meaning.
An
auditory
word
perception
deficit
can
be
identified
in
patients
that
have
a
deficit
in
word
repetition
that
cannot
be
attributed
to
a
more
general
articulatory
deficit.8'
It
can
be
assessed
through
phonological
discrimination
tasks
that
are
usually
included
in
most
of
the
traditional
aphasia
batteries.
Impairment
of
word
meaning
is
one
component
of
the
semantic
memory
disorders
(see
earlier)
and,
as
the
word
retrieval
deficit
(see
later),
can
be
category
specific.
For
example,
selective
deficits
for
abstract
and
concrete
concepts
and
within
the
concrete
domain
animate
or
inanimate
reference
and
even
specific
word
class
effects
have
all
been
reported.82
One
of
the
most
direct
tests
of
word
meaning
are
synonyms
tests
(for
example,
"timid"
means
"afraid"
or
"quiet").
Coughlan
and
Warrington83
have
offered
a
modest
standard-
isation
of
one
such
test.
Word
meaning
com-
prehension
can
also
be
tested
by
using
word-picture
matching
tests
such
as
the
pyra-
mids
and
palm
tree
test7l
and
the
British
picture
vocabulary
test,72
described
in
the
semantic
memory
section.
In
addition,
the
recent
psycholinguistic
assessment
of
lan-
guage
processing
in
aphasia84
is
a
useful
research
tool
for
assessing
comprehension
in
the
domains
of
verbal
and
visual
knowledge.
One
of
the
earliest
and
most
commonly
used
test
of
sentence
comprehension
is
the
token
test
devised
by
De
Renzi
and
Vignolo.85
This
test
uses
tokens
of
different
shapes,
sizes,
and
colours
and
the
patient
is
given
an
oral
instruction
in
progressively
more
com-
plex
non-redundant
sentences
(for
example,
"put
the
red
circle
between
the
yellow
square
and
the
green
square").
There
have
been
var-
ious
modifications
of
the
test
including
a
shortened
version
by
De
Renzi
and
Faglioni
and
a
very
abbreviated
version
by
Coughlan
and
Warrington.8'
Educationally
standardised
normative
data
are
available.
Parisi
and
Pizzamiglio86
devised
a
test
specifically
for
testing
syntactic
comprehension
(for
an
English
version
see
Lesser87).
Another
test
for
grammatical
comprehension
is
the
test for
reception
of
grammar.88
This
test
was
devel-
oped
for
the
assessment
of
language
develop-
ments
and
has
been
used
also
in
the
context
of
acquired
aphasia
investigations.
It
should
be
acknowledged
that
some
normative
data
are
available
for
the
sentence
comprehension
test
reviewed
here
and
are
undoubtedly
very
useful
for
in
depth
assessment
of
a
patient's
aphasic
deficit.
Word
and
sentence
retrieval-Word
retrieval
difficulties
are
exemplified
by
the
syndrome
of
amnestic
or
nominal
dysphasia
and
are
often
present
in
other
aphasic
syndromes
and
in
cortical
degenerative
conditions.
They
can
be
specific
for
particular
categories
such
as
letters,
colours,
body
parts,
proper
names,
and
fruits
and
vegetables.3'
To
evaluate
word
retrieval
difficulties
naming
from
verbal
description
(for
example,
"what
is
the
name
of
the
large
grey
animal
with
a
trunk")
and
picture
naming
tests
can
be
used.
The
graded
naming
test89
was
developed
to
identify
very
mild
degrees
of
anomia.
It
comprises
items
of
low
frequency
and
it
has
been
standardised
in
a
normal
population
and
validated
in
patients
with
unilateral
lesions.
The
Boston
naming
test90
comprises
line
drawings
of
objects
and
has
been
widely
used
in
aphasia
studies.
Only
a
limited
standardisation
is
available.
Spontaneous
language
is
often
elicited
by
complex
picture
description.
The
cookie
jar
theft
picture
from
the
Boston
diagnostic
aphasia
examination
is
widely
used
for
this
purpose.
De
Renzi
and
Ferrari9'
devised
the
reporter
test
requiring
the
patient
to
act
as
a
reporter
of
the
performance
carried
out
by
the
examiner
who
acts
in
accordance
with
the
commands
of
the
token
test
described
earlier.
This
test
is
particularly
useful
for
the
identifi-
cation
of
impairments
in
grammatical
sen-
tence
construction,
although
there
are
only
limited
normative
data
at
present.
WRItEN
LANGUAGE
In
the
past
30
years
cognitive
neuropsycholo-
gists
have
investigated
reading
and
writing
disorders
in
detail
and
depth.
This
has
resulted
in
the
identification
of
new
syn-
dromes
that
take
the
description
of
reading
and
writing
difficulties
well
beyond
the
classic
syndrome
described
by
Dejerine3:
dyslexia
with
dysgraphia
and
dyslexia
without
dys-
graphia.
Each
of
these
different
dyslexic
and
dysgraphic
syndromes
corresponds
to
an
identifiable
impairment
in
a
subcomponent
or
subcomponents
of
the
reading
and
writing
process.
Shallice
and
Warrington92
have
pro-
posed
a
distinction
between
peripheral
and
central
dyslexic
syndromes
and
this
dichotomy
applies
equally
well
to
the
dys-
graphia
syndromes.
Peripheral
dyslexias
and
dysgraphias
result
from
damage
to
processes
responsible
for
the
categorisation
of
a
string
of
letters
or
phonemes
as
orthographic
or
phonological
entities.
Central
dyslexias
and
dysgraphias
are
due
to
impairment
in
the
comprehension
and
production
of
a
target
stimulus.
The
study
of
central
dyslexias
and
dysgraphias
has
provided
evidence
that
there
are
at
least
two
parallel
forms
of
processing
for
reading
and
writing:
one
phonologically
based
and
one
semantic
based.
Phonological
processing
utilises
a
set
of
rules
for
translating
to
sound
or
sound
to
print.
It
is
used
for
reading
or
writ-
ing
unfamiliar
words
or
non-words.
Semantic
processing
accesses
meaningful
representa-
tions
of
the
words
that
are
in
the
subject's
vocabulary.
These
two
types
of
processing
can
break
down
independently
to
produce
different
types
of
reading
and
writing
impairments.
660
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
Neuropsychological
assessment
Assessment
of
written
language
Following
the
seminal
work
of
Marshall
and
Newcombe93
a
psycholinguistic
method
of
assessment
of
written
language
disorders
has
gained
wide
popularity.
This
method
involves
the
presentation
of
lists
of
words
that
sample
contrasting
psycholinguistic
properties.
It
is
thought
that
the
data
on
the
effect
of
the
psy-
cholinguistic
and
visual
(length,
script,
and
displays)
variables
coupled
with
an
errors
analysis
allow
conclusions
to
be
drawn
about
the
likely
origin
of
dysfunction
within
the
reading
system.
In
this
section
we
provide
a
description
of
some
of
the
standardised
and
validated
formal
tests
for
the
assessment
of
reading
and
spelling
disorders.
Reading-Any
assessment
of
reading
skills
should
include
an
evaluation
of
a
patient's
ability
to
read
both
single
words
and
text.
In
some
peripheral
dyslexias
the
ability
to
read
text
can
be
impaired
whereas
the
ability
to
read
single
words
can
be
spared.
(for
exam-
ple,
attentional
dyslexia94)
The
Neale
test95
for
assessment
of
prose
reading
in
children
is
useful
in
this
context.
In
addition,
most
of
the
standard
aphasia
batteries
include
a
subtest
for
text
reading
(for
example,
Boston
diag-
nostic
aphasia
examination).
For
the
formal
assessment
of
single
words,
the
two
most
widely
used
tests
are
the
NART
and
the
Schonell
graded
word
reading
test.
Both
tests
are
graded
in
difficulty
and
are
measures
of
reading
skills;
an
estimate
of
premorbid
opti-
mal
level
of
functioning
can
also
be
obtained
(see
earlier).
When
assessing
reading
skills
it
is
important
to
evaluate
the
patient's
ability
to
read
aloud
non-words.
This
ability
can
be
selectively
impaired
despite
good
word
read-
ing
as
in
the
case
of
phonological
dyslexia.96
No
formal
standardised
tests
for
non-word
reading
are
available;
however,
several
lists
have
been
devised
for
research
purposes.
Oral
and
written
spelling-Written
and
oral
spelling
are
known
to
dissociate
and
therefore
are
assessed
independently.
For
the
assess-
ment
of
written
spelling
the
Schonell
graded
spelling
test
can
be
used.
For
the
assessment
of
oral
and
written
spelling,
Baxter
and
Warrington97
have
recently
standardised
and
validated
a
test
that
is
sensitive
to
minor
degrees
of
deficit
in
the
general
neurological
population.
This
is
a
graded
difficulty
test;
thus
the
raw
scores
can
be
converted
into
per-
centile
scores.
For
patients
whose
poor
eye
sight
precludes
reading,
spelling
can
provide
a
useful
measure
of
premorbid
abilities.
The
assessment
of
non-word
spelling
is
also
important
because
patients
with
phonological
dysgraphia
might
present
with
some
pre-
served
word
spelling
despite
impaired
non-
word
spelling.98
Several
non-word
spelling
lists
have
been
developed
for
research
pur-
poses.
The
use
of
the
standard
word
reading
and
spelling
tests
described
combined
with
assess-
ment
of
the
patient's
ability
to
read
and
write
non-words
and
an
analysis
of
the
errors
made
by
the
patient
identifies
more
than
the
pres-
ence
of
a
reading
or
spelling
disorder.
It
can
also
provide
some
preliminary
indication
of
the
status
of
the
peripheral
and
central
pro-
cessing
involved
in
word
reading
and
spelling.
This,
rather
than
the
description
of
the
presence
or
absence
of
a
reading
or
spelling
disorder,
has
a
clear
clinical
and
theoretical
significance.
Purposes
of
a
neuropsychological
assessment
There
are
at
least
three
main
reasons
for
con-
ducting
a
neuropsychological
assessment:
diagnosis,
treatment
and
management,
and
research.
DIAGNOSIS
A
neuropsychological
assessment
allows
the
description
and
evaluation
of
the
major
cog-
nitive
deficits
incurred
in
neurological
patients
with
possible
brain
disease.
Furthermore,
it
can
indicate
possible
neuroanatomical
corre-
lates
of
the
cognitive
impairments.
A
neu-
ropsychological
assessment
can,
at
the
very
least,
provide
pointers
as
to
whether
there
is
unilateral,
bilateral,
or
subcortical
damage.
This
information
can
be
useful
in
diagnosis.
Neuropsychological
assessment
has
a
key
role
in
differentiating
between
organic
and
func-
tional
disorders.'4
99-101
There
are
other
neuro-
logical
conditions-for
example,
cortical
atrophy,
frontal
and
temporal
lobe
tumours,
and
undetected
temporal
lobe
seizures-that
may
manifest
themselves
with
symptoms
that
can
be
misinterpreted
as
functional.'02
For
example,
patients
with
visual
disorientation
disorders
due
to
bilateral
occipital
disease
are
often
misdiagnosed
on
the
grounds
that
their
visual
handicap
seems
to
be
disproportionate
in
the
context
of
normal
or
near
normal
acuity.
103
On
the
other
hand,
patients
with
symp-
toms
of
pseudodementia,
such
as
hysteria,
malingering,
Ganser
syndrome,
bipolar
disor-
der,
and
other
ill
defined
psychiatric
disorders
often
present
with
an
abrupt
intellectual
and
memory
failure
that
mimics
true
cognitive
deficits.
A
neuropsychological
assessment
can
distinguish
between
organic
and
functional
disorders.
It
does
this
by
highlighting
discrep-
ancies
between
subjective
complaints
and
objective
performance,
usually
detecting
a
number
of
inconsistencies
in
the
patients'
performance
and
a
too
obvious
mismatch
between
objective
performance
and
daily
life
activities.
Also,
the
body
of
neuropsychologi-
cal
knowledge
on
the
organisation
and
frac-
tionation
of
cognitive
skills
is
nowadays
highly
developed.
Crucially,
the
way
in
which
the
cognitive
functions
can
fractionate
often
diverges
from
the
common
sense
opinion
of
how
a
cognitive
function
can
break
down.
Thus
the
patient's
pattern
of
performance
can
be
interpreted
as
neuropsychologically
convncmig
or
unconvincing.
To
consider
one
example,
a
neuropsychological
assessment
is
useful
in
differentiating
organic
and
func-
tional
memory
loss.
Studies
of
patients
with
dense
organic
amnesia
have
shown
that
they
can
still
learn
new
associative
information
provided
that
they
are
tested
using
implicit
661
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
Cipolotti,
Warnngton
learning
tasks.
104
For
example,
they
show
savings
with
repeated
presentations
of
frag-
mented
or
degraded
stimuli
(pictures
or
words)
in
increasing
degrees
of
completeness.
Even
quite
severely
demented
patients
would
show
some
learning
with
these
tasks.
Clearly
a
patient
showing
additional
impairments
on
these
implicit
learning
tasks
makes
no
neuro-
psychological
sense.
It
makes good
common
sense,
however,
to
also
be
impaired
on
these
tasks
(I
have
poor
memory,
I
can't
remember
things).
Indeed,
a
poor
performance
on
these
tests
may
be
considered
indicative
of
func-
tional
memory
loss.
On
the
contrary,
a
rela-
tively
preserved
performance
on
these
tasks
conforms
to
an
organic
pattern.
Another
common
differential
diagnosis
where
neuropsychological
assessment
has
a
key
role
is
between
early
dementia,
anxiety
or
depressive
disorders,
or
the
normal
aging
process.
The
diagnosis
of
probable
dementia
is
usually
made
by
establishing
whether
there
is
an
acquired
deficit
of
cognition
without
hystopathological
evidence
obtained
from
a
biopsy
or
necropsy.
Often
in
the
early
stages
of
a
dementing
illness,
the
clinical
diagnosis
cannot
be
supported
by
neuroimaging
such
as
CT,
MRI,
or
functional
imaging.
Patients
with
depression
or
anxiety
may
complain
of
intellectual
or,
more
often,
memory
failure
similar
to
the
so
called
"worried
well"
patients.
Usually
depressive
or
anxiety
pseudodementia
should
be
suspected
when
the
patient
com-
plains
of
the
memory
problem
more
than
the
carer.'05
In
these
cases
recognition
memory
tests
should
be
used
to
determine
whether
the
failure
is
due
to
an
organic
condition
or
to
anx-
iety
or
depression.
It
has
been
shown
that
recall
tests
of
memory
are
vulnerable
to
the
effect
of
anxiety
and
depression,
whereas
recognition
memory
tests
are
not.'06
The
aging
process
itself
is
associated
with
cognitive
and
memory
changes.
Hence,
it
is
often
necessary
to
differentiate
memory
fail-
ure
due
to
cognitive
deterioration
rather
than
benign
senescent
forgetfulness.
In
these
cases
performance
in
recall
memory
tests
requiring
the
subjects
to
engage
in
elaborative
encod-
ing,
as
opposed
to
allowing
them
to
devise
their
own
encoding
strategy,
may
discrimi-
nate
those
with
brain
damage
from
normal
elderly
subjects.
For
example,
in
word
list
learning
the
strategy
of
performing
associa-
tions
between
successive
words
improves
the
overall
level
of
recall
in
normal
subjects.
Neuropsychological
assessment
can
also
have
a
central
role
in
diagnosing
presympto-
matic
cognitive
impairments
in
familial
neu-
rodegenerative
conditions.
From
the
nature
of
the
inheritance
and
the
relatively
constant
ages
of
disease
onset
within
a
family,
asymp-
tomatic
at
risk
subjects
below
the
mean
age
of
onset
can
be
examined.
Such
studies
have
shown
cognitive
abnormalities
in
apparently
asymptomatic
subjects
with
Huntington's
and
Alzheimer's
disease.33
107
TREATMENT
AND
MANAGEMENT
The
baselines
of
cognitive
functioning
pro-
vided
by
the
neuropsychological
examination
allow
the
monitoring
of
certain
conditions.
For
example,
successive
neuropsychological
examinations
provide
reliable
indications
of
whether
a
pattern
of
cognitive
deficit
associ-
ated
with
head
injury
or
stroke
is
changing
and
if
so,
how
rapidly
and
in
what
way.
This
information
is
useful
in
planning
the
future
medical
and
social
care
of
the
patient.
Similarly,
repeated-neuropsychological
testing
of
patients
with
degenerative
disorders
can
provide
information
about
their
different
rates
of
cognitive
decline
and
thus
help
them
and
their
family
plan
for
their
care.
The
results
of
a
neuropsychological
assessment
can
also
be
used
in
the
evaluation
of
medical
and
surgical
treatments
such
as
those
associ-
ated
with
subcortical
pathology
that
is
associ-
ated
with
cognitive
slowing
(for
example,
Parkinson's
disease
and
hydrocephalus).
For
instance,
obtaining
repeated
measures
of
a
hydrocephalic
patient's
performance
in
a
series
of
psychomotor
tests
can
provide
a
reli-
able
indication
of
whether
the
underlying
neurological
condition
is
improving
or
deteri-
orating.
Psychomotor
tests
are
simple
verbal
and
non-verbal
tests
that
involve
verbal
and
visuomotor
responses
and
the
measurement
of
the
patients'
speed
of
responding.
108
Practice
effects
are
minimal
in
these
tests,
which
are
at
the
same
time
sensitive
to
subtle
changes
in
cognitive
efficiency.
Therefore
they
can
be
used
at
regular
and
short
intervals
for
monitoring
the
patient's
neurological
state.
Neuropsychological
assessment
is
also
particularly
important
in
monitoring
the
vari-
ous
treatments
for
epilepsy.'09
110
The
baselines
of
cognitive
functioning
pro-
vided
by
the
neuropsychological
examination
can
be
important
for
planning
and
monitoring
rehabilitation
programmes.
In
particular,
when
planning
such
programmes,
neuro-
psychological
evaluation
can
provide
answers
to
key
questions
such
as
".
.
.
what
are
realis-
tic
treatment
goals
and
...
what
is
the
patient's
capacity
to
benefit
from
available
treatment
..
.".12
Moreover,
repeated
neu-
ropsychological
testing
can
be
used
to
monitor
the
effects
of
the
rehabilitation
programme.
Furthermore,
the
baselines
of
cognitive
func-
tioning
provided
by
neuropsychological
exam-
ination
can
be
used
to
explain
to
patients
and
their
families
their
relative
cognitive
problems
so
that
they
can
both
prepare
and
understand
the
type
of
difficulties
the
patient
may
face
when
he
leaves
the
hospital.
Neuropsychological
assessment
has
a
central
role
in
the
medicolegal
context.
Neuropsychological
data
concerning
the
type
and
severity
of
a
cognitive
deficit,
its
prognostic
value,
and
the
implications
for
future
care
are
central
issues
in
the
litigation
over
compensation
awards."'
In
this
context
neuropsychological
investigation
is
crucial
in
assessing
the
possibility
of
simulated
disability
that
can
sometimes
occur
before
financial
settlement.
"12
RESEARCH
There
are
two
main
neuropsychological
research
methodologies:
the
single
case
study
662
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
Neuropsychological
assessment
and
the
group
study.7
For
both,
neuropsycho-
logical
assessment
procedures
are
a
crucial
element.
The
strength
of
novel
findings
and
unexpected
dissociations
can
only
be
evalu-
ated
by
reference
to
performance
of
estab-
lished
tests
of
cognitive
skills.
In
group
studies
that
explore
new
hypotheses
neu-
ropsychological
measures
are
necessary
to
obtain
baseline
data.
Neuropsychological
research
may
consider
applied
clinical
problems
or
be
theoretically
driven.
In
applied
research,
batteries
of
neu-
ropsychological
tests
are
commonly
given
to
describe
the
cognitive
profiles
associated
with
particular
neurological
diseases.
For
example,
specific
cognitive
profiles
have
been
obtained
for
diseases
such
as
Parkinson's
disease
and
multiple
sclerosis.113
114
Neuropsychological
assessments
are
also
used
to
identify
specific
patterns
of
cognitive
deficits
associated
with
Alzheimer's
disease
and
other
degenerative
dementias.'
15-1
18
In
particular,
attempts
have
been
made
to
differentiate
various
subgroups
existing
within
a
category
of
dementing
disorders.
The
long
term
aim
is
to
further
the
understanding
of
the
disease,
in
particular
with
regard
to
early
diagnosis
and
the
possi-
bility
of
pharmacological
intervention."
19
120
Theoretically
driven
neuropsychological
research
has
proved
to
be
of
fundamental
importance
in
the
study
of
the
organisation
of
cerebral
functions
in
the
brain
and
especially
in
the
understanding
of
normal
cognitive
functioning.
Cognitive
neuropsychological
single
case
studies
have
been
used
as
a
legiti-
mate
type
of
evidence
to
support
or
criticise
information
processing
models
of
normal
cognition.
Furthermore
they
can
provide
valuable
findings
that
constrain
the
develop-
ment
of
new
theories.
Over
the
years
the
medical
literature
has
grown
immensely.
New
cognitive
deficits
have
been
identified
and
important
advances
have
been
made
in
the
understanding
of
the
relations
between
com-
ponents
of
complex
cognitive
skills
and
the
loci
of
brain
lesions.
The
neuropsychologist's
strategy
is
to
harness
and
incorporate
the
findings
of
research
investigations
to
a
clinical
problem
in
the
form
of
more
specific
and
more
sensitive
tests
of
cognitive
function.
These
improved
quantitative
techniques
for
clinical
assessment
can
bring
to
light
new
phenomena
that
in
turn
promote
funrther
the-
oretical
advances.
1
Broca
P.
Remarques
sur
le
siege
de
la
faculte
du
langage
articule
suivie
d'une
observation
d'aphemie.
Bulletin
de
la
Societe
d'Anatomie
Paris
1861;6:330.
(Translated
in
Herrnstein
R,
Boring
EG.
A
source
book
in
the
history
of
psychology.
Cambridge,
MA:
Harvard
University
Press,
1965.)
2
Wemicke
K.
Der
aphasische
Symptomenkomplex.
Breslau.
(Translated
in:
Boston
studies in
philosophy
of
science
1874;4:34-97.)
3
Dejerine
J.
Contribution
a
l'etude
anatomoclinique
et
clinique
des
differentes
varsetes
de
cecite
verbal.
CR
Hebdomadaire
des
Sceances
et
Memories
de
la
Societe
de
Biologie
1892;4:61-90.
4
Henschen
SE.
Klinische
und
anatomische
Betrag
zur
Pathologie
des
Gehirns.
Stockholm:
Nordiske
Bokhandein,
1920.
5
Lissauer
H.
Ein
Fall
von
Seelenblindheit
nebst
einem
Beitrag
zur
Theorie
derselben.
Archiv
fur
Psychiatrie
1890;21:222-270.
(Edited
and
reprinted
in
translation
by
M
Jackson
Lissauer
on
agnosia.
Cognitive
Neuropsychology
1988;5:155-192.)
6
Korsakoff
SS.
Uber
eine
besondere
Form
psychischer
Storung
combiniert
mit
multipler
Neuritis.
Archiv
fur
Psychiatrie
und
nervenkrankheiten
1889;21:669-704.
7
Shallice
T.
From
neuropsychology
to
mental
structure.
New
York:
Cambridge
University
Press,
1988.
8
Wechsler
DA.
Weschsler-Bellevue
intelligence
scales.
New
York:
Psychological
Corporation,
1939.
9
Wechsler
DA.
A
standardized
memory
scale
for
clinical
use.
JPsychol
1945;19:87-95.
10
Raven
JC.
Standard
progressive
matrices.
London:
H K
Lewis,
1938.
11
Raven
JC.
Coloured
progressive
matrices.
London:
H
K
Lewis,
1956.
12
Lezak
MD.
Neuropsychological
assessment.
2nd
ed.
New
York:
Oxford
University
Press,
1983.
13
Crawford
JR,
Parker
DM,
McKinlay
WW.
A
handbook
of
neuropsychological
assessment.
Hove,
UK:
Erlbaum,
1992.
14
Hodges
JR.
Cognitive
assessment
for
clinicians.
Oxford:
Oxford
University
Press,
1994.
15
McKenna
P,
Warrington
EK.
The
analytical
approach
to
neuropsychological
assessment.
In:
Grant
I,
Adams
KM,
eds.
Neuropsychological
assessment
of
neuropsychi-
atric
disorders.
New
York:
Oxford
University
Press,
1986:31-47.
16
Gall
F,
Spurzheim
G.
Research
on
the
nervous
system
in
general
and
on
that
of
the
brain
in
particular.
1809.
(Reprinted
in:
Pribram
K,
ed.
Brain
and
behaviour:
Vol
1.
Mood
states
and
mind.
Harmondsworth:
Penguin
Books,
1969.)
17
Bastian
HC.
Aphasia
and
other
speech
defects.
London:
Lewis,
1898.
18
Jackson
JH.
Case
of
large
cerebral
tumor
without
optic
neuritis
and
with
left
hemiplegia
and
imperception.
Royal
Opthalmological
Hospital
Reports
1876;8:434-44.
19
Lichtheim
L.
On
aphasia.
Brain
1885;7:433-84.
20
Freud
S.
Zur
Auffassung
der
Aphasien.
Vienna:
Deuticke,
1891.
21
Marie
P.
Revision
de
la
question
de
l'aphasie:
la
troisieme
convolution
frontale
gauche
ne
joue
aucun
role
speciale
dans
la
fonction
du
langage.
Semaine
Medicale
1906;21:241-7.
(Reprinted
in
Cole
MF,
Cole
M,
eds.
Pierre
Marie's
papers
on
speech
disorders.
New
York:
Hafner,
1971.)
22
Head
H.
Aphasia
and
kindred
disorders
of
speech.
Cambridge:
Cambridge
University
Press,
1926.
23
Goldstein
K.
Language
and
language
disturbances.
New
York:
Grune
and
Stratton,
1948.
24
Lashley
KS.
Brain
mechanisms
and
intelligence.
Chicago:
University
of
Chicago
Press,
1929.
25
Kandel
ER,
Schwartz
JH,
Jessell
TM.
Principles
of
Neural
Science.
3rd
ed.
New
York:
Elsevier,
1991.
26
Marr
D.
Vision.
San
Francisco,
CA:
Freeman,
1982.
27
Chomsky
N.
Rules
and
representations.
Behavioral
and
Brain
Sciences
1980;3:1-61.
28
Morton
J.
The
status
of
information
processing
models
of
language.
Philos
Trans
Roy
Soc
Lond
Biol
1981;295:
387-96.
29
Fodor
JA.
The
modularity
of
mind.
Cambridge,
MA:
MIT
Press,
1983.
30
Zeki
S,
Lamb
M.
The
neurology
of
kinetic
art.
Brain
1994;117:607-36.
31
McCarthy
RA,
Warrington
EK.
Cognitive
neuro-
psychology.
New
York:
Academic
Press,
1990.
32
McCloskey
M.
Cognitive
mechanisms
in
numerical
pro-
cessing:
evidence
from
acquired
dyscalculia.
Cognition
1992;44:
107-57.
33
Newman
SK,
Warrington
EK,
Kennedy
AM,
Rossor
MN.
The
earliest
cognitive
change
in
a
person
with
familial
Alzheimer's
disease:
presymptomatic
neu-
ropsychological
features
in
a
pedigree
with
familial
Alzheimer's
disease
confirmed
at
necropsy.
J
Neurol
Neurosurg
Psychiatry
1994;57:967-72.
34
Matarazzi
JD.
Wechsler's
measurement
and
appraisal
of
adult
intelligence.
5th
ed.
Baltimore:
Williams
and
Wilkins,
1972.
35
Crawford
JR.
Current
and
premorbid
intelligence
mea-
sures
in
neuropsychological
assessment.
In:
Crawford
JR,
Parker
DM,
McKinley
WW,
eds.
A
handbook
of
neuropsychological
assessment.
Hove,
UK:
Erlbaum,
1992:21-49.
36
Yates
AJ.
The
use
of
vocabulary
in
the
measurement
of
intellectual
deterioration.
Journal
of
Mental
Science
1956;102:409-40.
37
Nelson
HE,
McKenna
P.
The
use
of
current
reading
ability
in
the
assessment
of
dementia.
Br
J
Soc
Clin
Psychol
1975;14:259-67.
38
Schonell
F.
Backwardness
in
the basic
subjects.
London:
Oliver
and
Boyd,
1942.
39
Nelson
HE,
O'Connell
A.
Dementia:
the
estimation
of
premorbid
intelligence
levels
using
the
new
adult
read-
ing
test.
Cortex
1978;14:234-44.
40
Stem
W.
Psychologische
Methoden
der
Intelligenz-Pnufung.
Leipzig:
Barth,
1912.
41
Nelson
HE.
The
national
adult
reading
test.
Windsor:
NFER-Nelson,
1991.
42
Grober
E,
Sliwinski
M.
Development
and
validation
of
a
model
for
estimating
premorbid
verbal
intelligence
in
the
elderly.
J
Clin
Exp
Neuropsychol
1991;13:933-49.
43
Patterson
K,
Graham
N,
Hodges
JR.
Reading
in
Alzheimer's
type
dementia:
a
preserved
ability?
Neuropsychology
1994;8:395-402.
44
Spearman
C.
The
abilities
of
man.
New
York:
Macmillan,
1927.
45
Thurstone
LL.
Primary
mental
abilities.
Chicago:
University
of
Chicago
Press,
1938.
663
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
Cipolotti,
Wayrington
46
Guilford
JP.
The
structure
of
intellect.
Psychol
BuUl
1956;53:267-93.
47
Cattell
RB.
The
measurement
of
adult
intelligence.
Psychol
Bull
1943;40:
153-93.
48
Raven
JC.
Advanced
progressive
matrices,
sets
I
and
II.
London:
H
K
Lewis,
1965.
49
Carpenter
PA,
Just
MA,
Shell
P.
What
one
intelligence
test
measures:
a
theoretical
account
of
the
processing
in
the
Raven
progressive
matrices
test.
Psychol
Rev
1990;97:404-31.
50
Bolin
BJ.
A
comparison
of
Raven's
progressive
matrices
(1938)
with
the
ACE
psychological
examination
and
the
Otis
gamma
mental
ability
test.
Consult
Psychol
1955;19:400.
51
Wechsler
DA.
The
Wechsler
adult
intelligence
scale:
manual.
New
York:
Psychological
Corporation,
1955.
52
Wechsler
DA.
The
measurement
and
appraisal
of
adult
intelligence.
4th
ed.
Baltimore:
Williams
and
Wilkins,
1958.
53
Bornstein
RA,
Matarazzo
JD.
Wechsler
VIQ
versus
PIQ
differences
in
cerebral
dysfunction:
a
literature
review
with
emphasis
on
sex
differences.
Journal
of
Clinical
Neuropsychology
1982;4:319-34.
54
Warrington
EK,
James
M,
Maciejewski
C.
The
WAIS
as
a
lateralising
and
localising
diagnostic
instrument:
a
study
of
656
patients
with
unilateral
cerebral
lesions.
Neuropsychologia
1986;24:223-239.
55
Ryan
nT,
Paolo
AM,
Brungardt
TM.
Standardization
of
the
Wechsler
adult
intelligence
scale-revised
for
per-
sons
75
years
and
older.
Psychological
Assessments:
A
Journal
of
Consulting
and
Clinical
Psychology
1990;
2:408-11.
56
Baddeley
AD.
Human
memory
theory
and
practice.
Hove,
UK:
Erlbaum,
1990.
57
Schacter
DL.
Implicit
memory:
history
and
current
sta-
tus.
JExp
Psychol
Gen
1987;13:501-18.
58
Warrington
EK,
Weiskrantz
L.
Amnesia:
consolidation
or
retrieval?
Nature
1970;228:628-30.
59
Tulving
E.
Episodic
and
semantic
memory.
In:
Tulving
E,
Donaldson
W,
eds.
Organization
of
memory.
New
York:
Academic
Press,
1972:382-404.
60
Corsi
P.
Interhemispheric
differences
in
the
localization
of
psychological
processes
in
man
[thesis].
University
of
Montreal, Montreal,
1972.
61
Gathercole
SE,
McCarthy
RA.
Memory
tests
and
tech-
niques.
Hove,
UK:
Erlbaum,
1994.
62
Coughlan
AK,
Hollows
SE.
The
adult
memory
and
infor-
mation
processing
battery.
St
James's
University
Hospital,
Leeds:
A
K
Coughlan,
1985.
63
Wilson
BA,
Cockburn
J,
Baddeley
AD.
The
Rivermead
behavioural
memory
test.
Bury
St
Edmunds,
UK:
Thames
Valley
Test
Company,
1985.
64
Rey
A.
L'examen
clinique
en
psychologie.
Paris:
Presses
Universitaires
de
France,
1964.
65
Osterreith
PA.
Le
test
de
copie
d'une
figure
complexe.
Archives
de
Psychologies
1944;30:206-356.
66
Benton
AL.
The
revised
visual
retention
test.
New
York:
Psychological
Corporation,
1974.
67
Warrington
EK.
Recognition
memory
test.
Windsor,
UK:
NFER-Nelson,
1984.
68
Clegg
F,
Warrington
EK.
Four
easy
memory
tests
for
older
adults.
Memory
1994;2:167-182.
69
Kopelman
MD,
Wilson
BA,
Baddeley
AD.
The
autobio-
graphical
memory
interview:
a
new
assessment
of
auto-
biographical
and
personal
semantic
memory
in
amnesic
patients.
Clin
Exp
Neuropsychol
1989;11:
724-44.
70
Warrington
EK.
The
selective
impairment
of
semantic
memory.
QJExpPsychol
1975;27:635-57.
71
Howard
D,
Patterson
KE.
Pyramids
and
palm
trees.
Bury
St
Edmunds,
UK:
Thames
Valley
Test
Company,
1992.
72
Dunn
LM,
Dunn
LM,
Whetton
C,
Pintilie
D.
The
British
picture
vocabulary
scale.
Windsor,
UK:
NFER-
Nelson,
1982.
73
Lesser
R.
Linguistic
investigations
of
aphasia.
London:
Arnold,
1978.
74
Gainotti
G,
Caltagirone
C,
Ibba
A.
Semantic
and
phone-
mic
aspects
of
auditory
language
comprehension
in
aphasia.
Linguistics
1975;154/155:15-29.
75
Miceli
G,
Gainotti
G,
Caltagirone
C,
Masullo
C.
Some
aspects
of
phonological
impairment
in
aphasia.
Brain
Lang
1980;11:159-169.
76
Goodglass
H,
Kaplan
E.
The
assessment
of
aphasia
and
related
disorders.
Philadelphia,
PA:
Lea
and
Febiger,
1972.
77
Kertesz
A.
Westen
aphasia
battery.
San
Antonio:
The
Psychological
Corporation
Inc,
1982.
78
Porch
BE.
The
Porch
index
of
communicative
ability-
theory
and
development.
Palo
Alto,
USA:
Consulting
Psychologists
Press,
1967
(revised
1971).
79
De
Bleser
R,
Bayer
J,
Luzzatti
C.
Die
cognitive
Neuropsychologie
der
Schriftsprache.
Ein
Uberblick
zwei
deutscher
Fallbeschreibungen.
In:
Bayer
J,
ed.
Grammatik
und
Cognition.
Linguistische
Berichte,
Suppl
_
1.
Wiesbaden:
Westdeutscher
Verlag,
1987:.118-162.
80 Caramazza
A.
The
logic
of
neuropsychological
research
and
the
problem
of
patient
classification
in
aphasia.
Brain
Lang
1984;21:9-20.
81
Auerbach
SH,
Allard
T,
Naeser
M,
Alexander
MP,
Albert
ML.
Pure
word
deafness:
an
analysis
of
a
case
with
bilateral
lesions
and
a
deficit
at
the
pre-phonemic
level.
Brain
1982;105:271-300.
82
McCarthy
RA,
Warrington
EK.
Disorders
of
semantic
memory.
Philos
Trans
Roy
Soc
Lond
Biol
1994;346:
89-96.
83
Coughlan
AK,
Warrington
EK.
Word-comprehension
and
word-retrieval
in
patients
with
localised
cerebral
lesions.
Brain
1978;101:163-85.
84
Kay
J,
Lesser
R,
Coltheart
M.
PALPA
psycholinguistic
assessments
of
language
processing
in
aphasia.
Hove,
UK:
Erlbaum,
1992.
85
De
Renzi
E,
Vignolo
LA.
The
token
test:
a
sensitive
test
to
detect
receptive
disturbances
in
aphasia.
Brain
1962;85:665-78.
86
Parisi
D,
Pizzamiglio
L.
Syntactic
comprehension
in
aphasia.
Cortex
1970;6:204-15.
87
Lesser
R.
Verbal
comprehension
in
aphasia:
an
English
version
of
three
Italian
tests.
Cortex
1974;10:
247-63.
88
Bishop
D.
Test
for
reception
of
grammar.
Oxford:
Thomas
Leach,
1982.
89
McKenna
P,
Warrington
EK.
The
graded
naming
test.
Windsor,
UK:
NFER-Nelson,
1980.
90
Kaplan
E,
Goodglass
H,
Weintraub
S.
Boston
naming
test
Philadelphia:
Lea
and
Febinger,
1983.
91
De
Renzi
E,
Ferrari
C.
The
reporter's
test:
a
sensitive
test
to
detect
expressive
disturbances
in
aphasics.
Cortex
1978;14:279-93.
92
Shallice
T,
Warrington
EK.
Single
and
multiple
compo-
nent
central
dyslexic
syndromes.
In:
Coltheart
M,
Patterson
KE,
Marshall
JC,
eds.
Deep
dyslexia.
London:
Routledge
and
Kegan
Paul,
1980:119-45.
93
Marshall
JC,
Newcombe
F.
Patterns
of
paralexia:
a
psy-
cholinguistic
approach.
Psycholinguist
Res
1973;2:
175-99.
94
Warrington
EK,
Cipolotti
L,
McNeil
J.
Attentional
dyslexia:
a
single
case
study.
Neuropsychologia
1993;31:
871-85.
95
Neale
MD.
The
Neale
analysis
of
reading
ability.
2nd
ed.
London:
Macmillan,
1966.
96
Beauvois
M-F,
Derouesne
J.
Phonological
alexia:
three
dissociations.
Y
Neurol
Neurosurg
Psychiatry
1979;42:
1115-24.
97
Baxter
DM,
Warrington
EK.
Measuring
dysgraphia:
a
graded-difficulty
spelling
test
(GDST).
Behavioural
Neurology
1994;71:107-16.
98
Shallice
T.
Phonological
agraphia
and
the
lexical
route
in
writing.
Brain
1981;104:413-29.
99
Miller
E.
Cognitive
assessment
of
the
older
adult.
In:
Birren
JE,
Sloane
RB,
eds.
Handbook
of
mental
health
and
ageing.
Engelwood
Cliffs,
NJ:
Prentice-Hall,
1980.
100
Kendrick
DC.
Kendrick
cognitive
tests
for
the
elderly.
Windsor,
UK:
NFER-
Nelson,
1985.
101
Caine
ED.
Pseudodementia:
current
concepts
and
future
directions.
Arch
Gen
Psychiatry
198
1;38:1359-64.
102
Strub
RL,
Black
FW.
Mental
status
examination
in
neurol-
ogy.
2nd
ed.
Philadelphia,
PA:
Davis,
1985.
103
Godwin-Austin
RB.
A
case
of
visual
disorientation.
Neurol
Neurosurg
Psychiatry
1965;28:453-8.
104
Warrington
EK,
Weiskrantz
L.
New
method
of
testing
long-term
retention
with
special
reference
to
amnesic
patients.
Nature
1968;277:972-4.
105 Rossor
MN.
Management
of
neurological
disorders:
dementia.
Neurol
Neurosurg
Psychiatry
1994;57:
1451-6.
106
Coughlan
AK,
Hollows
SE.
The
use
of
memory
tests
in
differentiating
organic
disorder
from
depression.
Br
Psychol
1984;145:164-7.
107
Bennett
T,
Curiel
M.
Early
neuropsychological
presenta-
tion
of
Huntington's
disease.
Int
7
Clin
Neuropsychol
1989;11:90-5.
108
Willison
JR,
Warrington
EK.
Cognitive
retardation
in
a
patient
with
preservation
of
psychomotor
speed.
Behavioural
Neurology
1992;5:113-6.
109
Goldstein
H.
Neuropsychological
investigation
of
tempo-
ral
lobe
epilepsy.
J
R
Soc
Med
1991;84:460-5.
110
Jones-Gotman
M.
Localization
of
lesions
by
neuropsy-
chological
testing.
Epilepsia
1991;32(suppl
5):S41-52.
111
McKinlay
WW.
Assessment
of
the
head-injured
for
com-
pensation.
In:
Crawford
JR,
Parker
DM,
McKinlay
WW,
eds.
A
handbook
of
neuropsychological
assessment.
Hove,
UK:
Erlbaum,
1992:381-92.
112
Miller
H.
Mental
after
effects
of
head
injury.
Proceedings
of
the
Royal
Society
of
Medicine
1966;59:257-61.
113
Lees
J,
Smith
E.
Cognitive
deficits
in
the
early
stages
of
Parkinson's
disease.
Brain
1983;106:257-270.
114
Grant
I,
McDonald
WI,
Trimble
MR,
Smith
E,
Reed
R.
Deficient
learning
and
memory
in
early
and
middle
phases
of
multiple
sclerosis.
Y
Neurol
Neurosurg
Psychiatry
1984;47:250-5.
115
Cummings
J,
Benson
D,
Read
S.
Aphasia
in
dementia
of
the
Alzheimer
type.
Neurology
1985;35:394-7.
116
Spinnler
H,
Della
Sala
S.
The
role
of
clinical
neuropsy-
chology
in
the
neurological
diagnosis
of
Alzheimer's
disease.
Neurol
1988;235:258-71.
117
Haxby
J,
Grady
C,
Koss
E,
et
al.
Longitudinal
study
of
cerebral
metabolic
asymmetries
and
associated
neu-
ropsychological
patterns
in
early
dementia
of
the
Alzheimer-type.
Arch
Neurol
1990;47:753-60.
118
Swearer
J,
O'Donnell
B,
Drachman
D,
Woodward
B.
Neuropsychological
features
of
familial
Alzheimer's
disease.
Ann
Neurol
1992;32:687-94.
119
Martin
A.
Neuropsychology
of
Alzheimer's
disease:
the
case
for
subgroups.
In:
Schwartz
M,
ed.
Modular
deficits
in
Alzheimer
type
dementia.
Cambridge,
Mass:
MIT
Press,
1990:143-75.
120
Rossor
MN,
Kennedy
AK,
Newman
S.
Heterogeneity
of
familial
Alzheimer's
disease.
In:
Boller
F,
Forette
F,
Khachaturian
Z,
eds.
Heterogeneity
of
Alzheimer's
dis-
ease.
Berlin:
Springer-Verlag,
1992:81-7.
664
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
doi: 10.1136/jnnp.58.6.655
1995 58: 655-664 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
L Cipolotti and E K Warrington
Neuropsychological assessment.
http://jnnp.bmj.com/content/58/6/655.citation
Updated information and services can be found at:
These include:
service
Email alerting the box at the top right corner of the online article.
Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article. Sign up in
Notes
http://group.bmj.com/group/rights-licensing/permissions
To request permissions go to:
http://journals.bmj.com/cgi/reprintform
To order reprints go to:
http://group.bmj.com/subscribe/
To subscribe to BMJ go to:
group.bmj.com on July 15, 2011 - Published by jnnp.bmj.com Downloaded from
... Aerobic exercise has been proposed as effective in improving working memory score of the T2DM participants, if they are not having brain damage 21 . Perhaps, T2DM individuals pass through stages of neuronal changes in specific brain regions such as the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, which explained reasons behind cognitive deficits 22,23 . ...
... In patients with T2DM, the functional connectivity between the hippocampus and other brain regions gets reduced 24,25 which results in decline in cognitive performance in T2DM, which is associated with a reduction in functional connectivity 24 . T2DM individuals if do not face problems of any memory failure, they may not have any cognitive deterioration 21 . In these cases, as Cipolotti, & Warrington 21 postulated, these T2DM individuals could engage in elaborative encoding, and could develop their own encoding strategy, in order to enhance in their level of working memory 22,23 . ...
... In these cases, as Cipolotti, & Warrington 21 postulated, these T2DM individuals could engage in elaborative encoding, and could develop their own encoding strategy, in order to enhance in their level of working memory 22,23 . Combined introduction of exercise training regime may lead to higher-order cognitive competence, in the form of enhancement in visual motor integration and configurational ability 21 . ...
... outcome. As spelled out by Cipolotti and Warrington (1995), "objective" methods to document how "… brain damage can selectively disrupt some components of a cognitive system (p. 655 )" represent critical reference points for establishing brain-behavior relations. ...
... Accordingly, these are the candidate ROIs to volumetrically examine in mTBI as depicted in this "cone of vulnerability." These regions, quantified volumetrically, would also meet the Cipolotti and Warrington (1995) standard for neuropsychology by providing an "objective" metric for making neuropsychological inference to brain structure and function and demonstrating how "damage" may alter neuropsychological outcome. ...
- Erin D. Bigler
Region of interest (ROI) volumetric assessment has become a standard technique in quantitative neuroimaging. ROI volume is thought to represent a coarse proxy for making inferences about the structural integrity of a brain region when compared to normative values representative of a healthy sample, adjusted for age and various demographic factors. This review focuses on structural volumetric analyses that have been performed in the study of neuropathological effects from mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) in relation to neuropsychological outcome. From a ROI perspective, the probable candidate structures that are most likely affected in mTBI represent the target regions covered in this review. These include the corpus callosum, cingulate, thalamus, pituitary-hypothalamic area, basal ganglia, amygdala, and hippocampus and associated structures including the fornix and mammillary bodies, as well as whole brain and cerebral cortex along with the cerebellum. Ventricular volumetrics are also reviewed as an indirect assessment of parenchymal change in response to injury. This review demonstrates the potential role and limitations of examining structural changes in the ROIs mentioned above in relation to neuropsychological outcome. There is also discussion and review of the role that post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) may play in structural outcome in mTBI. As emphasized in the conclusions, structural volumetric findings in mTBI are likely just a single facet of what should be a multimodality approach to image analysis in mTBI, with an emphasis on how the injury damages or disrupts neural network integrity. The review provides an historical context to quantitative neuroimaging in neuropsychology along with commentary about future directions for volumetric neuroimaging research in mTBI.
... The identification of brain-behavior relationships is a central aim of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. Such investigations are predicated on fundamental assumptions in the field: (a) the cerebral cortex possesses a high degree of specialization, (b) brain function can be inferred via a modularity approach to analyzing complex cognitive skills, and (c) brain damage can selectively disrupt subcomponents of a cognitive system (Cipolotti & Warrington, 1995). Investigating prefrontal cortex modularity of EF in nonpatient samples is a prerequisite for understanding the nature of those systems under neuropathological circumstances. ...
... The neural correlates found for the D-KEFS in this study support the unitary-diverse dialectic of EF. Our findings are consistent with the evidence that brain volume is related to neuropsychological ability, but also further distinguishes how the brain and behavior are related (Cipolotti & Warrington, 1995). The aggregate of D-KEFS scores was more disparately associated with prefrontal volumes, whereas individual D-KEFS scores were more narrowly but strongly related to specific ROIs. ...
Objective: Designed to measure a diversity of executive functioning (EF) through classical neuropsychological tests, the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function Scale (D-KEFS) allows for the investigation of the neural architecture of EF. We examined how the D-KEFS Tower, Verbal Fluency, Design Fluency, Color-Word Interference, and Trail Making Test tasks related to frontal lobe volumes, quantifying the regional specificity of EF components. Method: Adults from the Nathan Kline Institute-Rockland Sample (NKI-RS), an open-access community study of brain development, with complete MRI (3T scanner) and D-KEFS data were selected for analysis (N = 478; ages 20-85). In a mixed-effects model predicting volume, D-KEFS task, D-KEFS score, region of interest (ROI; 13 frontal, 1 occipital control), were entered as fixed effects with intercepts for participants as random effects. Results: "Unitary" EF (aggregate of D-KEFS scores) was positively associated with superior frontal, rostral middle frontal, and lateral orbitofrontal volumes; a negative association was observed with frontal pole volume (| z-score slope | range = 0.040 to 0.051). "Diverse" EF skills (individual D-KEFS task scores) were differentially associated with two or three ROIs, respectively, but to a stronger extent (| z-score slope | range = 0.053 to 0.103). Conclusions: The neural correlates found for the D-KEFS support the prefrontal modularity of both unitary (aspects of EF ability common to all tasks) and diverse EF. Our findings contribute to emerging evidence that aggregate measurements of EF may serve broader but less robust frontal neural correlates than distinct EF skills. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
... 灵活性涵盖许多不同的类型 [9~11] , 认知灵活性作为 执行功能(executive function)的重要组成部分已受到研 究者的广泛关注. 认知灵活性这一概念最早被定义为 根据情况变化的需求产生或变换反应方式的能力 [12] , 关于执行功能的研究已经对认知灵活性在心理方面的 作用和发展进行了系统阐述 [13,14] . [22] . ...
... A hypothesis testing approach is common in which a series of tests are given, guided by a clinical and cognitive conceptual framework (Cipolotti and Warrington, 1995;Luria, 1972;Shallice, 1988). First, it is assumed that there is a high degree of functional specialization within the cerebral cortex. ...
A neuropsychological assessment aims to determine the impact of a known or suspected brain-related condition on thinking skills (cognition), behavior and mood. This is undertaken by clinical interview and via administration of a set of psychometric scales and tests designed to ascertain current level of function and detect changes, impairments, problems or symptoms experienced by the individual in any aspect of cognition or behavior, or in mood. The purpose may be to assist with neurological diagnosis, to identify, monitor or manage neuropsychological impairments, or to devise rehabilitation management strategies or interventions.
- Cheng Sun
- Yunsong Han
- Lin Luo
- Huixuan Sun
The study aimed to assess the effect of indoor air temperature on cognitive work performance of acclimatized people during the middle of heating season in severely cold region in China. How does temperature affect work performance was also investigated through subjective surveys. Eighteen acclimatized and healthy participants (10 males and 8 females) carried out tasks in a climate chamber under six temperatures (18°C, 20°C, 22°C, 24°C, 26°C and 28°C). They completed 11 neuro-behavioural tests, including perception, learning and memory, thinking, expression and executive function tests, and subjective surveys on thermal sensation, emotion, health, workload and enthusiasm. Results showed that temperature has an effect on the variation on the neuro-behavioural performance depending on task types and difficulties. Temperature could influence the response time significantly more than accuracy when tasks were performed without feedback. Under adverse environment, people would feel (slightly) cool or warm, feel less enthusiastic to work, and would have more negative emotions, perceived sick building syndrome to be more serious and would have evaluated performance as worse. They would also perform badly on neuro-behavioural tests. Subjective surveys were important supplements to objective evaluation on the effect of indoor environment factors on work performance.
- Sheena McDonald
- Allan Little
- Gail A Robinson
Rebuilding Life after Brain Injury: Dreamtalk tells the survival story of Sheena McDonald, who in 1999 was hit by a police van and suffered a very severe brain injury. Sheena's story is told from her own, personal standpoint and also from two further unique and invaluable perspectives. Allan Little, a BBC journalist and now Sheena's husband, describes both the physical and mental impact of the injury on himself and on Sheena. Gail Robinson, Sheena's neuropsychological rehabilitation specialist, provides professional commentaries on Sheena's condition, assessment and recovery process. The word Dreamtalk, created by Allan to describe Sheena's once "hallucinogenic state", sets the tone for this book. It humanises and contextualises the impact of brain injury, providing support and encouragement for patients, professionals and families. It presents exclusive insights into each stage of recovery, spanning coma, altered consciousness, post-traumatic amnesia and rehabilitation; all showing how she has defied conventional clinical expectations and made an exceptional recovery. This book is valuable reading to those who have suffered a brain injury and also to professionals such as neurologists, neuropsychologists, physiotherapists, occupational therapists and speech therapists working in the field. © 2019 Sheena McDonald, Allan Little & Gail Robinson. All rights reserved.
- T.L. Bennett
- M. P. Curiel
A dissociation between choreoathetoid movements and cognitive impairment is demonstrated through case presentation of two women with early symptoms of Huntington's Disease. The two individuals showed strikingly similar patterns of impairment on an extended Halstead-Reitan Battery. However, one exhibited prominent choreoathetoid movements, and the other showed none. Performance on tests that depended on fine motor speed, either directly or adjunctly, was significantly impaired. Logical analysis and flexibility of thinking were also compromised. Memory impairment, while present, while present, was not as pronounced as that reported by previous investigators. None of the MMPI clinical scales were significantly elevated. Deviations in the present study from earlier reports were related to duration of symptoms prior to assessment. The data reported in this paper were collected as part of the initial diagnostic process.
- L. L. Thurstone
This publication is the opening number of a series which the Psychometric Society proposes to issue. It reports the first large experimental inquiry, carried out by the methods of factor analysis described by Thurstone in The Vectors of the Mind 1. The work was made possible by financial grants from the Social Science Research Committee of the University of Chicago, the American Council of Education, and the Carnegie Corporation of New York. The results are eminently worthy of the assistance so generously accorded. Thurstone's previous theoretical account, lucid and comprehensive as it is, is intelligible only to those who have a knowledge of matrix algebra. Hence his methods have become known to British educationists chiefly from the monograph published by W. P. Alexander8. This enquiry has provoked a good deal of criticism, particularly from Professor Spearman's school ; and differs, as a matter of fact, from Thurstone's later expositions. Hence it is of the greatest value to have a full and simple illustration of his methods, based on a concrete inquiry, from Professor Thurstone himself.
- Martin N Rossor
- A M Kennedy
- S. K. Newman
Alzheimer's original description in 1907 portrayed a 51-year-old woman who died following a dementing illness and who was found to have senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles throughout the cerebral cortex. Her clinical features were those of profound memory impairment and language and visuospatial deficits. There was no family history reported.
- Rosaleen Anne McCarthy
- Elizabeth K. Warrington
This chapter presents an introduction to cognitive neuropsychology. The term cognitive neuropsychology is applied to the analysis of those handicaps in human cognitive function that result from brain injury. Cognitive neuropsychology is essentially interdisciplinary, drawing both on neurology and on cognitive psychology for insights into the cerebral organization of cognitive skills and abilities. Cognitive function is the ability to use and integrate basic capacities such as perception, language, actions, memory, and thought. The focus of clinical cognitive neuropsychology is on the many different types of highly selective impairments of cognitive function that are observed in individual patients following brain damage. The functional analysis of patients with selective deficits provides a very clear window through which one can observe the organization and procedures of normal cognition. Clinical cognitive neuropsychology has been successful in demonstrating a large number of dissociations between the subcomponents of cognitive skills. This enables to conclude that such components are dependent on distinct neural systems.
In diesem Beitrag stellen wir zwei Fälle mit unterschiedlichen erworbenen Störungen der Schriftsprache vor, eine Patientin mit „Tiefendyslexie" (§ 3.1) und einen Patienten mit „Oberflächendysgraphie" (§ 3.2).
Source: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/15589804_Neuropsychological_assessment
Posted by: quentincaissiee016157.blogspot.com
Posting Komentar